Among The Most Thorough Olopatadine Guide Book You Ever Seen Or Else Your Money Back

Материал из Wiki
Перейти к:навигация, поиск

5). This pattern again suggests that participants use both lexicosyntactic and prosodic cues for turn-projection. As in Experiment 1, when both cues were incomplete, participants were least likely to expect a speaker change, whereas when both cues were complete, they were the most likely to expect one. When the cues were pitted against each other, listeners weighed lexicosyntactic over prosodic cues. Figure 5 Proportion of baseline (dashed) and actual anticipatory (solid) gaze switches to the answerer, by condition and age. The conditions were: Fully incomplete (?SYN ?PROS); Incomplete syntax (?SYN +PROS); Incomplete prosody (+SYN ?PROS); ... Lexicosyntactic and prosodic cues A model similar to that fitted in Experiment 1 assessed the effects of linguistic cues and age on participants' baseline-corrected Cilengitide mw anticipatory gaze switches (1144 observations, N = 40; Table ?Table7).7). The dependent variable was participants' baseline-corrected anticipatory switches. Predictor variables included syntactic completeness (incomplete vs. complete), prosodic completeness (incomplete vs. complete) and age (toddler vs. adult). The intercept was allowed to vary by subject and item, and the predictor variables were contrast-coded (?1, 1). Table 7 Outcomes from main linear mixed effects Non-specific serine/threonine protein kinase model for both subject groups (English toddlers and adults; Number of observations: 1144, N = 40). Again we found that the amount of linguistic information consistent with turn completion affected participants' anticipatory switching (Figure ?(Figure6).6). Model coefficients show four significant effects (Figure IWR1 ?(Figure6;6; Table ?Table7).7). First, the proportion of anticipatory gaze switches was larger for the lexicosyntactically complete vs. lexicosyntactically incomplete targets (�� = 0.17, z = 10.55, p